Section 1: Introduction to Lifespan Development

1.5 Theoretical Perspectives and Theories of Development

Who are the major theorists and theories organizing the study of development?

Learning Objectives
  • Differentiate between theories and hypotheses
  • Differentiate between theoretical perspectives and theories
  • Describe the major theoretical perspectives and the corresponding theories as they relate to lifespan development.

As we have learned, human development refers to the physical, cognitive, and psychosocial changes and constancies in humans over time. There are various theories pertaining to each domain of development, and often, theorists and researchers focus their attention on specific periods of development (with most traditionally focusing on infancy and childhood, some on adolescence). However, development during one period can affect development in other periods, and humans can grow and change throughout their lifespan. 

What is a Theoretical Perspective or Orientation?

People who study lifespan development approach it from different perspectives. Each perspective encompasses one or more theories—the broad, organized explanations and predictions concerning phenomena of interest. These groups of theories make up a theoretical orientation or theoretical perspective.  You may see either of these terms used in multiple Psychology textbooks for the same thing: categorizing theories.  Theories of development provide a framework for thinking about human growth, development, and learning. If you have ever wondered about what motivates human thought and behavior, understanding these theories can provide useful insight into individuals and society.

Theories can be developed using induction in which a number of single cases are observed, and after patterns or similarities are noted, the theorist develops ideas based on these examples. Established theories are then tested through research; however, not all theories are equally suited to scientific investigation. Some theories are difficult to test but are still useful in stimulating debate or providing concepts that have practical applications.  Keep in mind that theories are not facts; they are guidelines for investigation and practice, and they gain credibility through research that fails to disprove them.

What is a theory?

In lifespan development, we need to rely on a systematic approach to understanding behavior based on observable events and the scientific method.  There are so many different observations about childhood, adulthood, and development in general that we use theories to help organize all of the different observable events or variables.

A theory guides and helps us interpret research findings. It provides the researcher with a blueprint or model to help piece together various studies. Theories are guidelines; think of them as instructions that come with an appliance or other object that requires assembly. The instructions can help someone piece together smaller parts more easily than trial and error.

A theory is a simplified explanation of the world that attempts to explain how variables interact with each other. It can take complex, interconnected issues and narrow them down to the essentials, enabling developmental theorists and researchers to analyze the problem in greater depth.

A cycle with four stages: Theory, Hypothesis, Research, Observation. A Theory is used to form a hypothesis. A hypothesis is used to design a study to test the hypothesis. This brings us to the research stage. Performing the research leads to the observation stage which leads to the creation or modification of the theory. This leads back to the Theory stage, and the cycle repeats.
Figure 1. Theories are often revisited and tested through experiments and research.

Two key concepts in the scientific approach are theory and hypothesis (Figure 1). A theory is a well-developed set of ideas that propose an explanation for observed phenomena that can be used to make predictions about future observations. A hypothesis is a testable prediction that is arrived at logically from a theory. It is often worded as an if-then statement (e.g., if I study all night, I will get a passing grade on the test). The hypothesis is extremely important because it bridges the gap between the realm of ideas and the real world. As specific hypotheses are tested, theories are modified and refined to reflect and incorporate the results of these tests. In essence, lifespan theories explain observable events in a meaningful way. They are not as specific as hypotheses, which are so specific that we use them to make predictions in research. Theories offer more general explanations of behavior and events.

History of Developmental Psychology

Early psychologists, including Freud and Hall.
Figure 2. Some major players in the early development of psychology. Front row: Sigmund Freud, G. Stanley Hall, Carl Jung. Back row: Abraham A. Brill, Ernest Jones, Sándor Ferenczi, at Clark University in Worcester, Massachusetts. Date: September 1909.

The scientific study of children began in the late nineteenth century. It blossomed in the early twentieth century as pioneering psychologists sought to uncover the secrets of human behavior by studying its development. Developmental psychology made an early appearance in a more literary form, however. William Shakespeare’s melancholy character, “Jacques” (in As You Like It), articulated the “seven ages of man,” which included three stages of childhood and four of adulthood.

Two early scholars, John Locke and Jean-Jacques Rousseau proposed theories of human behavior that are the direct ancestors of the major theoretical traditions of developmental psychology today (Vasta et al., 1998, p. 10). Locke, a British empiricist, adhered to a strict environmentalist position that the mind of the newborn is a tabula rasa (“blank slate”) on which knowledge is written through experience and learning. Rousseau, a Swiss philosopher who spent much of his life in France, proposed a nativistic model in his famous novel Emile, in which development occurs according to innate processes progressing through three stages: Infans (infancy), puer (childhood), and adolescence. Rousseau detailed some of the necessary progression through these stages in order to develop into an ideal citizen. Although some aspects of his text were controversial, Rousseau’s ideas were strongly influential on educators at the time.

Developmental theories can be divided into a few general camps: psychodynamic, behavioral, cognitive, humanistic, evolutionary, and systems theories. Each theory and its proponents will be briefly reviewed. You can also check out the other chapters in this section for detailed information about each theoretical perspective. Videos at the end provide more in-depth coverage of each theory.  As we cover the entire lifespan, each theory will be covered in greater detail. At this point, it’s helpful to start learning the main ideas, players, and what they say about how we develop. Which theory most closely aligns with your views on human development?

Theoretical Orientations (Perspective) and Theories

Psychodynamic Perspective

The Psychodynamic Orientation or Perspective contains theories that focus on unconscious conflicts and drives. Unconscious refers to parts of our mind that we are unaware of. Originally referred to as psychoanalytic theories, the term psychodynamic reflects modern interpretations. Two of the most prominent psychoanalytic theorists were Sigmund Freud and Erik Erikson.

Psychosexual Theory

Sigmund Freud holding a cigar
Figure 3. Sigmund Freud

A name you are probably familiar with who was influential in the study of human development is Sigmund Freud (1856-1939). Sigmund Freud’s stage theory of psychosexual development grew out of his psychoanalytic approach to human personality and psychopathology. Freud based his model of child development on his own and his patients’ recollections of their childhood. He developed a stage model of development in which the libido, or sexual energy, of the child focuses on different “zones” or areas of the body as the child grows to adulthood. Freud’s model is “interactionist” since he believed that although the sequence and timing of these stages are biologically determined, successful personality development depends on the experiences the child has during each stage. Although the details of Freud’s developmental theory have been widely criticized for lacking evidence and being difficult to test, his emphasis on the importance of early childhood experiences prior to five years of age has had a lasting impact.

Freud emphasized the importance of early childhood experiences in shaping our personality and behavior. In our natural state, we are biological beings. We are driven primarily by instincts. During childhood, however, we begin to become social beings as we learn how to manage our instincts and transform them into socially acceptable behaviors. The type of parenting the child receives has a very powerful impact on the child’s personality development. We will explore this idea further in our discussion of psychosexual development, but first, we must identify the parts of the “self” in Freud’s model, or in other words, what constitutes a person’s personality and makes us who we are.

Read more about Psychosexual Theory in Appendix A: Psychodynamic Perspective–Psychoanalysis

Psychosocial Theory

Erik Erikson
Figure 4. Erik Erikson

Erik Erikson (1902-1994) also adopted the psychoanalytic approach, but he diverged from Freud by focusing more on social influences in personality development. Instead of a psychosexual model, Erikson proposed eight stages of psychosocial development: trust versus mistrust, autonomy vs. shame/doubt, initiative vs. guilt, identity vs. role confusion, intimacy vs. isolation, generativity vs. stagnation, and integrity vs. despair. Each stage included a conflict or developmental task to progress through each stage. His theory covered the entire lifespan, from infancy through late adulthood, as opposed to Freud, whose last stage ended after adolescence. Erikson’s stages catalyzed research into social, emotional, and identity development, but it was criticized for lacking adequate detail on how people resolve each crisis. People may not progress through these stages in the same order, and some stages may be limited to certain cultural and historical contexts. However, many find his stages to be a useful summary of social tasks across the lifespan.

Read more about Psychosocial Theory in Appendix A: Psychodynamic Perspective–Erikson and Psychosocial Theory.

Behavioral and Social Cognitive Perspective

In sharp contrast to the more subjective approach espoused by Freud and Erikson, behaviorism, or learning theory, sought to establish psychology as a rigorous scientific discipline by only focusing on observable behaviors. Behaviorism emphasizes the role of nurture, or the environment, in human development. Behaviorists only studied actions and what you could see, essentially neglecting the mind. They used human and animal models to demonstrate how learned behaviors could be gradually acquired, which was consistent with non-stage theories.

Classical Conditioning

Ivan Pavlov
Figure 5. Ivan Pavlov

The Russian physiologist Pavlov (1849-1936) did not consider himself a psychologist: he worked on digestion in dogs, which led to a Nobel Prize. He discovered the theory of classical conditioning almost by accident when working with dogs in his lab. He noticed that every time he fed his dogs, they would salivate. Over time, the dogs started salivating even before the food arrived, such as when they saw the lab assistant approach or heard their footsteps coming down the hallway. This led Pavlov to investigate how we can learn automatic reactions by associating two stimuli together, which is classical conditioning. Although classical conditioning explains a small subset of developmental behaviors, it’s influential in learned emotional responses and paved the way for behavioral theories.

Watson (1878-1958) was the founder of behaviorism and expanded upon Pavlov’s research. Based on Locke’s environmentalist position, he believed that human behavior can be understood in terms of experiences and learning. He believed that all behaviors are learned or conditioned, as evidenced by his famous “Little Albert” study, in which he classically conditioned an infant to fear a white rat.

John B. Watson
Figure 6. John B. Watson

In Watson’s book, Psychological Care of Infant and Child in 1928, he explained that children should be treated as young adults—with respect, but also without emotional attachment. In the book, he warned against the inevitable dangers of a mother providing too much love and affection. Watson explained that love, along with everything else as the behaviorist saw the world, is conditioned. Watson supported his warnings by mentioning invalidism, saying that society does not overly comfort children as they become young adults in the real world, so parents should not set up these unrealistic expectations. His book became highly criticized for being cold, and his research and personal life for ethical lapses (he had an extramarital affair with his graduate assistant, Rosalie Rayner), but he was still influential in promoting more research into early childhood behavior and development.

Read more about Classical Conditioning in Appendix A: Behavioral Perspective–Classical and Operant Conditioning.

Operant Conditioning

B.F. Skinner
Figure 7. B.F. Skinner

According to a 2002 survey, another behaviorist, B.F. Skinner was considered the most eminent psychologists of the 20th century (Haggbloom et al., 2002). Like other behaviorists, he argued for nurture and the environment in shaping development. Skinner thought that all behavior was learned and proposed a theory of operant conditioning, which is familiar to any parent or child. Our behavior is shaped by consequences, rewards, and punishments, which Skinner calls reinforcement and punishment. If the consequences of your action make your behavior more likely to happen in the future, then it’s considered reinforcement. If the consequences make your behavior less likely, then it’s punishment. If you cram for an exam and do well, you’re likely to cram again in the future, so that cramming behavior is reinforced. However, if you cram for unit exams but then forget everything for the final exam, you recognize the strategy is ineffective for long-term learning, and you (hopefully) become less likely to cram in the future. That’s punishment. Skinner is also known for some idiosyncratic applications of operant conditioning, such as temperature-controlled cribs for babies and pigeon-guided precision bombs (GIY – Google it yourself, for more info).

Read more about Operant Conditioning in Appendix A: Behavioral Perspective–Classical and Operant Conditioning.

Social Cognitive Theory (Social Learning)

Albert Bandura wearing glasses
Figure 8. Albert Bandura

Bandura (1925-2021) extended behavioral theory by considering social and cognitive influences. He was not a radical behaviorist like Watson or Skinner, but most famous for his social cognitive theory and Bobo doll experiments. Bandura believed that children learn by observing others, which is also referred to as social learning, observational learning, or modeling.

Read more about Social Learning Theory in Appendix A: Behavioral and Social Cognitive Perspective–Observational Learning.

Cognitive Perspective

Cognitive is another word for “thinking.” The Cognitive orientation contains theories focused on the mind and how thinking develops.

Piaget’s Cognitive Development Theory

Piaget wearing glasses
Figure 9. Jean Piaget

Jean Piaget (1896-1980) ranked behind Skinner as the second most eminent psychologist of the 20th century, and his stage theory of cognitive development revolutionized our view of children’s thinking and learning. His interest lies in children’s knowledge, their thinking, and the qualitative differences in their thinking as it develops. Although he called his field “genetic epistemology,” stressing the role of biological determinism, he also assigned great importance to experience. In his view, children “construct” their knowledge by building schemas or mental models.

A black cat lies on its side while a red dog looks at it
Figure 10. Young kids may develop different schemas or mental categories for cats and dogs.

To Piaget, knowledge grows through processes of “assimilation,” in which people evaluate and try to understand new information based on their existing knowledge of the world, and “accommodation,” in which they expand and modify their mental models based on new experiences. If a young child has a pet cat, they develop a schema that cats are typically four-legged, furry, and meow. When they see a new cat that has a different color coat, they assimilate it into their current cat category or schema. However, if they see a furry, four-legged creature that barks, they may mistakenly identify it as a cat but be corrected that it’s a dog. They need to accommodate their schema, or mental categories, and create a new category for dogs.

Piaget proposed four stages of cognitive development from infancy through adolescence, and his work inspired more research than any other theorist. Many of his concepts are still foundational to developmental psychology. More recent cognitive psychologists found that Piaget underestimated the cognitive capabilities of infants and children and overestimated the abilities of adolescents. Piaget was also criticized for his stage approach, which doesn’t capture all the individual differences in cognitive development.

Learn more about Piaget’s theory in Appendix A: Cognitive Theories–Piaget and Information Processing.

Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Theory

Lev Vygotsky is wearing a suit
Figure 11. Lev Vygotsky

Although Lev Vygotsky was born in the same year as Piaget, he died at age 37 of tuberculosis. He was a Russian psychologist interested in how children’s thinking developed. He and Piaget both believed in the importance of the child being actively involved in their learning, but Vygotsky differed from Piaget in that his sociocultural theory focused more on social influences and how cultural tools transform one’s thinking. Vygotsky did not propose a series of stages like Piaget. Two of Vygotsky’s concepts, the zone of proximal development and scaffolding, are still influential in education circles. The zone of proximal development refers to the skills or abilities that a novice can accomplish if they have help from a more skilled person. Scaffolding, like the construction term, refers to temporary supports that more skilled people provide to help children advance in their cognitive and physical skills.

Learn more about this theory in Appendix A: Cognitive Perspective: Sociocultural Cognitive Theory.

Information Processing

Information processing theory is a set of cognitive theories that emerged from the cognitive revolution of the 1950s and 60s. They focus on how information “flows” through the mind and often adopt a computer metaphor of the mind. As the name suggests, the central processes of the mind are attention, memory, reasoning, problem-solving, and planning. Although the computer metaphor is limited, most modern cognitive theories of development adopt an information-processing approach when investigating how different thinking processes change over time. When we learn more about how memory changes in childhood and late adulthood or how modern technology affects attention, we’ll consider the information processing perspective.

Learn more about Information Processing theory in Appendix A: Cognitive Theories–Piaget and Information Processing.

Evolutionary and Ethological Perspectives 

Evolutionary Theory

Charles Darwin
Figure 12. Charles Darwin

The work of Charles Darwin, the British biologist famous for his theory of evolution, has been applied to studying human development. The human species shares a long evolutionary history, and that history, over thousands of years, has influenced how we think and act today across the lifespan. Ways of thinking and behaving that helped our ancestors survive and reproduce were more often passed down through generations. Evolutionary theories are helpful for tackling many of the “why” questions of human behavior, such as why we think and act the way we do. For example, why do humans have one of the longest childhood periods of any mammal? From an evolutionary view, extending childhood for so long is risky in delaying reproductive age because one could die before that age. But that risk could be outweighed by the benefit of developing a large brain capable of learning complex social skills (Machluf et al., 2014). In short, our advanced human social skills, which take a lengthy childhood to develop, promote survival and reproduction. Evolutionary theories are important for their theoretical contributions but are notoriously difficult to test.

Ethological Theory

The evolutionary perspective draws heavily on the field of ethology, which examines the ways in which our biological makeup influences our behavior. The primary proponent of ethology was Konrad Lorenz, who discovered that newborn geese are genetically pre-programmed to become attached to the first moving object they see after birth. Lorenz’s work led developmentalists to consider the ways in which human behavior might reflect inborn genetic patterns. Working with geese, he investigated the principle of imprinting, the process by which some nidifugous birds (i.e. birds that leave their nest early) bond instinctively with the first moving object that they see within the first hours of hatching. Although Lorenz did not discover the topic, he became widely known for his descriptions of imprinting as an instinctive bond.

John Bowlbly used the basis of Ethological theory to develop the significance of attachment in humans.  He proposed that attachment to a caregiver in the first years of life has a significant impact throughout the lifespan. A secure attachment results in optimal development, while insecure and negative attachments will result in developmental deficits. We will explore the theory of attachment in more depth later in the text.

Learn more about these theories in Appendix A: Evolutionary and Ethological Perspectives.

Ecological Perspective

Bronfenbrenner’s Ecological Systems Theory

Another psychologist who recognized the importance of the environment on development was American psychologist Urie Bronfenbrenner (1917-2005), who formulated the ecological systems theory to explain how the inherent qualities of a child and their environment interact to influence how they will grow and develop. The term “ecological” refers to a natural environment; human development is understood through this model as a long-lasting transformation in the way one perceives and deals with the environment. Bronfenbrenner’s ecological theory stresses the importance of studying children in the context of multiple environments because children typically find themselves enmeshed simultaneously in different ecosystems. Each of these systems inevitably interacts with and influences each other in every aspect of the child’s life, from the most intimate level to the broadest. Furthermore, he eventually renamed his theory the bioecological model in order to recognize the importance of biological processes in development. However, he only recognized biology as producing a person’s potential, with this potential being realized or not via environmental and social forces. Those environmental contexts can be conceptualized as different systems.

An individual is impacted by microsystems such as parents, siblings, or friends, those who have direct, significant contact with the person. The influence is bidirectional, meaning the person influences their friends and is influenced by them. The relationships between people surrounding the person also influence them. This is considered the mesosystem: the forces and connections between the people surrounding the child. A good example is the relationship between parents or the relationship between a parent and the child’s teacher. The mesosystem has also been interpreted to reflect the interactions between systems. The next level is the larger context of the community, referred to as the exosystem. The exosystem includes the social settings that do not directly involve the child, such as community structures, transportation, mass media, local industries, hospitals, and school systems. The community is influenced by the macrosystem, which is the overarching cultural context, such as religious beliefs, political systems, global economic conditions, war, technological trends, values, philosophies, and a society’s responses to the global community. In sum, a child’s experiences are shaped by larger forces such as the family, neighborhood, culture, and the interactions among them. Remember, the influence is bidirectional: children also shape their families and other contexts. All of this occurs within the relevant historical context and timeframe, or chronosystem. The chronosystem refers to time and considers any changes in the child and the environment over time, including any historical events. The chronosystem consists of all the experiences that a person has had during their lifetime.

Learn more about Ecological Systems Theory in Appendix A: Ecological Perspective-Brofenbrenner.

 

Bronfenbrenner's bio ecological systems theory
Figure 13. Brofenbrenner’s ecological theory emphasizes the influence of the microsystem, mesosystem, exosystem, macrosystem, and chronosystem on an individual.

Humanistic Perspective

Emerging around the same time as information processing theories, the humanistic perspective contained theories focused on the positive aspects of human nature. Whereas prior developmental theories, like psychoanalytic and behavioral, thought free will was an illusion, humanistic theories embraced free will and the power of personal choice. Prominent humanistic theories, like Abraham Maslow, proposed a hierarchy of needs with self-actualization at the top. Self-actualization is the motivation to achieve our highest potential. According to Maslow’s hierarchy of needs, humans can live their best lives when their lower needs are met, including basic needs, safety, love and belonging, and esteem. Carl Rogers emphasized the importance of humans receiving unconditional positive regard to optimize their development: all people should be afforded dignity and respect without conditions. These theories have been immensely influential in therapy and education but are criticized for lacking scientific rigor.

Learn more about the Humanistic perspective in Appendix A: Humanistic Perspective–Theories of Rogers and Maslow.

Comparing and Evaluating Lifespan Theories

Developmental theories provide a set of guiding principles and concepts that describe and explain human development. Some developmental theories focus on the formation of a particular quality, such as Piaget’s theory of cognitive development. Other developmental theories focus on growth that happens throughout the lifespan, such as Erikson’s theory of psychosocial development. It would be natural to wonder which of the perspectives provides the most accurate account of human development, but clearly, each orientation or perspective is based on its own premises and focuses on different aspects of development.  Many lifespan developmental psychologists use an eclectic or integrative approach, drawing on several orientations/perspectives at the same time because the same developmental phenomenon can be looked at from a number of perspectives.

Throughout the course, we’ll explore and apply the major theories in greater depth. The goal right now is to introduce you to different ways of thinking about the lifespan and some of the advantages and limitations of each approach.  Recall the major issues considered in human development. Examine whether development is continuous or discontinuous (non-stage or stage), whether it is the same for everyone or distinct for individuals (one course of development or many) and whether it is more influenced by nature or by nurture.

Key Terms
  • theoretical orientation or perspective: a category for a group of theories that encompass the same basic principles.
  • hypothesis: a testable prediction
  • theory: a well-developed set of ideas that propose an explanation for observed phenomena that can be used to make predictions about future observations

Attributions

Human Growth and Development by Ryan Newton is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License,

Individual and Family Development, Health, and Well-being by Diana Lang, Nick Cone; Laura Overstreet, Stephanie Loalada; Suzanne Valentine-French, Martha Lally; Julie Lazzara, and Jamie Skow is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike 4.0 International License,

 

 

License

Icon for the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License

Lifespan Development Copyright © 2024 by Jennifer Ounjian is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License, except where otherwise noted.