Skill Practice: Understanding Graphs
Graphs are a useful way to explain information and are common in academic texts. Carefully analyzing graphic material, such as tables, pie charts, bar and line graphs, will not only help you understand academic texts more completely but will also provide you with additional, important information.
Here are some tips for interpreting graphs in your university texts:
Preview a graph before reading the section where it is located.
- What is the main idea of the graph?
- What is the purpose of the graph? Is it comparing things? If so, what?
- What can you predict that this section will be about?
- Read the title and labels so that you understand the point of the graph.
- What do the vertical and horizontal axes state?
- What do the different colors represent?
Read the relevant text carefully and check your predictions. Normally, the text will draw your attention to the figure at a point where it relates to the text. For example, it might include (Figure 5) when the written information corresponds to the picture or graph. Ask yourself:
- What text describes or is related to the figure?
- Were your predictions correct?
- What did you learn after reading the text and re-examining the graph?
Practice
Study the bar graph in Figure 3 from “Reading 2” and answer the following questions. Remember it’s often difficult to understand a graph without reading the corresponding text. Still, take a look at the graph below and try to answer the questions.
Try to answer these questions:
- What is the topic of the bar graph?
- What do the different colors/patterns represent?
- What do you think this graph shows?
Now, read the paragraph in “Reading 2” that comes before the Quiz Master graph. Check to see if your answers are correct.
Pre-Reading
Pre-Reading Activity
Complete the chart below. Brainstorm how situations and one’s own personality influence us.
Situational Influences |
Personality/Dispositional Influences |
How formal the situation is
|
How shy or outgoing a person is
|
Reading 2: Concepts of Social Psychology [1]
Situational and Dispositional Influences on Behavior
Behavior is a product of both the situation (e.g., cultural influences, social roles, and the presence of bystanders) and of the person (e.g., personality characteristics). Subfields of psychology tend to focus on one influence or behavior over others. Situationism is the view that our behavior and actions are determined by our immediate environment and surroundings. In contrast, dispositionism holds that our behavior is determined by internal factors (Heider, 1958). An internal factor is an attribute of a person and includes personality traits and temperament. Social psychologists have tended to take the situationist perspective, whereas personality psychologists have promoted the dispositionist perspective. Modern approaches to social psychology, however, take both the situation and the individual into account when studying human behavior (Fiske, Gilbert, & Lindzey, 2010). In fact, the field of social-personality psychology has emerged to study the complex interaction of internal and situational factors that affect human behavior (Mischel, 1977; Richard, Bond, & Stokes-Zoota, 2003).
Fundamental Attribution Error
In the United States, the predominant culture tends to favor a dispositional approach in explaining human behavior. Why do you think this is? We tend to think that people are in control of their own behaviors, and, therefore, any behavior change must be due to something internal, such as their personality, habits, or temperament. According to some social psychologists, people tend to overemphasize internal factors as explanations—or attributions—for the behavior of other people. They tend to assume that the behavior of another person is a trait of that person, and to underestimate the power of the situation on the behavior of others. They tend to fail to recognize when the behavior of another is due to situational variables, and thus to the person’s state. This erroneous assumption is called the fundamental attribution error (Ross, 1977; Riggio & Garcia, 2009). To better understand, imagine this scenario: Greg returns home from work, and upon opening the front door his wife happily greets him and inquires about his day. Instead of greeting his wife, Greg yells at her, “Leave me alone!” Why did Greg yell at his wife? How would someone committing the fundamental attribution error explain Greg’s behavior? The most common response is that Greg is a mean, angry, or unfriendly person (his traits). This is an internal or dispositional explanation. However, imagine that Greg was just laid off from his job due to company downsizing. Would your explanation for Greg’s behavior change? Your revised explanation might be that Greg was frustrated and disappointed for losing his job; therefore, he was in a bad mood (his state). This is now an external or situational explanation for Greg’s behavior.
The fundamental attribution error is so powerful that people often overlook obvious situational influences on behavior. A classic example was demonstrated in a series of experiments known as the quizmaster study (Ross, Amabile, & Steinmetz, 1977). Student participants were randomly assigned to play the role of a questioner (the quizmaster) or a contestant in a quiz game. Questioners developed difficult questions to which they knew the answers, and they presented these questions to the contestants. The contestants answered the questions correctly only 4 out of 10 times (Figure 3). After the task, the questioners and contestants were asked to rate their own general knowledge compared to the average student. Questioners did not rate their general knowledge higher than the contestants, but the contestants rated the questioners’ intelligence higher than their own. In a second study, observers of the interaction also rated the questioner as having more general knowledge than the contestant. The obvious influence on performance is the situation. The questioners wrote the questions, so of course they had an advantage. Both the contestants and observers made an internal attribution for the performance. They concluded that the questioners must be more intelligent than the contestants.
As demonstrated in the example above, the fundamental attribution error is considered a powerful influence in how we explain the behaviors of others. However, it should be noted that some researchers have suggested that the fundamental attribution error may not be as powerful as it is often portrayed. In fact, a recent review of more than 173 published studies suggests that several factors (e.g., how well hypothetical events are explained) play a role in determining just how influential the fundamental attribution error is (Malle, 2006).
Is the Fundamental Attribution Error a Universal Phenomenon?
You may be able to think of examples of the fundamental attribution error in your life. Do people in all cultures commit the fundamental attribution error? Research suggests that they do not. People from an individualistic culture, that is, a culture that focuses on individual achievement and autonomy, have the greatest tendency to commit the fundamental attribution error. Individualistic cultures, which tend to be found in western countries such as the United States, Canada, and the United Kingdom, promote a focus on the individual. Therefore, a person’s disposition is thought to be the primary explanation for her behavior. In contrast, people from a collectivistic culture, that is, a culture that focuses on communal relationships with others, such as family, friends, and community (Figure 4), are less likely to commit the fundamental attribution error (Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Triandis, 2001).
Why do you think this is the case? Collectivistic cultures, which tend to be found in Asian, Latin American, Arab and African countries, focus on the group more than on the individual (Nisbett, Peng, Choi, & Norenzayan, 2001). This focus on others provides a broader perspective that considers both situational and cultural influences on behavior; thus, a more nuanced explanation of the causes of others’ behavior becomes more likely. Table 1 summarizes compares individualistic and collectivist cultures.
Table 1 Characteristics of Individualistic and Collectivistic Cultures |
|
Individualistic Culture |
Collectivistic Culture |
Achievement oriented |
Relationship oriented |
Focus on autonomy |
Focus on group autonomy |
Dispositional perspective |
Situational perspective |
Independent |
Interdependent |
Analytic thinking style |
Holistic thinking style |
Actor-Observer Bias
Returning to our earlier example, Greg knew that he lost his job, but an observer would not know. So, a naïve observer would tend to attribute Greg’s hostile behavior to Greg’s disposition rather than to the true, situational cause. Why do you think we underestimate the influence of the situation on the behaviors of others? One reason is that we often don’t have all the information we need to make a situational explanation for another person’s behavior. The only information we might have is what is observable. Due to this lack of information we tend to assume the behavior is due to a dispositional, or internal, factor. When it comes to explaining our own behaviors, however, we have much more information available to us. If you came home from school or work angry and yelled at your dog or a loved one, what would your explanation be? You might say you were very tired or feeling unwell and needed quiet time—a situational explanation. The actor-observer bias is the phenomenon of attributing other people’s behavior to internal factors (fundamental attribution error) while attributing our own behavior to situational forces (Jones & Nisbett, 1971; Nisbett, Caputo, Legant, & Marecek, 1973; Choi & Nisbett, 1998). As actors of behavior, we have more information available to explain our own behavior. However, as observers, we have less information available; therefore, we tend to default to a dispositionist perspective.
One study on the actor-observer bias investigated reasons male participants gave for why they liked their girlfriend (Nisbett et al., 1973). When asked why participants liked their own girlfriend, participants focused on qualities of their girlfriends (for example, her pleasant personality) that are external to their own wellbeing. The participants’ explanations rarely included causes internal to themselves, such as dispositional traits (for example, “I need companionship.”). In contrast, when speculating why a male friend likes his girlfriend, participants were equally likely to give internal and external explanations. This supports the idea that actors tend to provide few internal explanations but many external, situational explanations for their own behavior. In contrast, observers tend to provide more dispositional explanations for a friend’s behavior (Figure 5).
Self-Serving Bias
Following an outcome, self-serving biases are those attributions that enable us to see ourselves in favorable light (for example, making internal attributions for success and external attributions for failures). When you do well at a task, for example acing an exam, it is in your best interest to make a dispositional attribution for your behavior (“I’m smart,”) instead of a situational one (“The exam was easy,”). The tendency of an individual to take credit by making dispositional or internal attributions for positive outcomes but situational or external attributions for negative outcomes is known as the self-serving bias (Miller & Ross, 1975). This bias serves to protect self-esteem. You can imagine that if people always made situational attributions for their behavior, they would never be able to take credit and feel good about their accomplishments.
We can understand self-serving bias by digging more deeply into attribution, a belief about the cause of a result. One model of attribution proposes three main dimensions: locus of control (internal versus external), stability (stable versus unstable), and controllability (controllable versus uncontrollable). In this context, stability refers the extent to which the circumstances that result in a given outcome are changeable. The circumstances are considered stable if they are unlikely to change. Controllability refers to the extent to which the circumstances that are associated with a given outcome can be controlled. Obviously, those things that we have the power to control would be labeled controllable (Weiner, 1979).
Consider the example of how we explain our favorite sports team’s wins. Research shows that we make internal, stable, and controllable attributions for our team’s victory (Figure 6) (Grove, Hanrahan, & McInman, 1991). For example, we might tell ourselves that our team is talented (internal), consistently works hard (stable), and uses effective strategies (controllable). In contrast, we are more likely to make external, unstable, and uncontrollable attributions when our favorite team loses. For example, we might tell ourselves that the other team has more experienced players or that the referees were unfair (external), the other team played at home (unstable), and the cold weather affected our team’s performance (uncontrollable).
Just-World Hypothesis
One consequence of westerners’ tendency to provide dispositional explanations for behavior is victim blame (Jost & Major, 2001). When people experience bad fortune, others tend to assume that they somehow are responsible for their own fate. A common ideology, or worldview, in the United States is the just-world hypothesis. The just-world hypothesis is the belief that people get the outcomes they deserve (Lerner & Miller, 1978). In order to maintain the belief that the world is a fair place, people tend to think that good people experience positive outcomes, and bad people experience negative outcomes (Jost, Banaji, & Nosek, 2004; Jost & Major, 2001). The ability to think of the world as a fair place, where people get what they deserve, allows us to feel that the world is predictable and that we have some control over our life outcomes (Jost et al., 2004; Jost & Major, 2001). For example, if you want to experience positive outcomes, you just need to work hard to get ahead in life.
Can you think of a negative consequence of the just-world hypothesis? One negative consequence is people’s tendency to blame poor individuals for their plight. What common explanations are given for why people live in poverty? Have you heard statements such as, “The poor are lazy and just don’t want to work” or “Poor people just want to live off the government”? What types of explanations are these, dispositional or situational? These dispositional explanations are clear examples of the fundamental attribution error. Blaming poor people for their poverty ignores situational factors that impact them, such as high unemployment rates, recession, poor educational opportunities, and the familial cycle of poverty (Figure 7). Other research shows that people who hold just-world beliefs have negative attitudes toward people who are unemployed and people living with AIDS (Sutton & Douglas, 2005). In the United States and other countries, victims of sexual assault may find themselves blamed for their abuse. Victim advocacy groups, such as Domestic Violence Ended (DOVE), attend court in support of victims to ensure that blame is directed at the perpetrators of sexual violence, not the victims.
Reading Comprehension
Complete the summary below using the words in the box.
biased collectivistic dispositional external fundamental attribution error internal individualistic self-esteem situational social psychology |
(1)___________________________________ is the subfield of psychology that studies the power of the situation to influence individuals’ thoughts, feelings, and behaviors. Psychologists categorize the causes of human behavior into two groups. First, those resulting from (2)_________________________ factors, such as personality. Second, those caused by (3)_________________________ factors, such as cultural and other social influences. Behavior is better explained, however, by using both approaches. People who aren’t psychologists tend to over-rely on (4)_________________________ explanations for behavior. They often ignore the power of (5)_________________________ influences. This perspective is called the (6)______________________________________________________________. People from (7)_________________________ cultures are more likely to display this bias. Whereas people from (8)_________________________ cultures are less likely to do so. Our explanations for others’ behaviors can be (9)_________________________ due to not having enough information about the motivation for their behaviors. Moreover, we tend to provide explanations for our own behaviors that bolster our (10)_________________________.
Choose the best answer to the following questions.
- Taking personal credit for your successes and blaming others for failures is an example of ________.
- actor-observer bias
- fundamental attribution error
- self-serving bias
- just-world hypothesis
- Collectivistic cultures are to ________ as individualistic cultures are to ________.
- dispositional; situational
- situational; dispositional
- autonomy; group harmony
- just-world hypothesis; self-serving bias
- According to the actor-observer bias, we have more information about ________.
- influences on our own behavior
- influences on others’ behavior
Answer the question in your own words.
- Using examples, explain whether you are from an individualist culture or collectivist culture? Explain your answer.
- Study the bar graph in Figure 5 from “Reading 2” and answer the following questions.
-
- What is the topic of the bar graph?
- What do the different colors/patterns represent?
- What are internal/external factors? Internal/external to the girlfriend or the respondent?
- What does this graph show?
Vocabulary Practice
Complete the sentences with the best word. You may need to change the verb form to fit the sentence.
advocate attribute bond consequences erroneous hypothetical label (v) oriented phenomenon scenarios |
- The group _________________________for equal pay for women by talking to lawmakers and producing public campaigns.
- After publishing _________________________ information, the newspaper issued a correction.
- The students’ actions had severe _________________________ as they were expelled from school.
- The storm was considered a natural _________________________ and not something that could be planned for.
- The chemical was _________________________ toxic after further testing.
- Children form a strong _________________________ with their parents.
- In their business class, the students discussed several _________________________, which they might encounter at work, and what they might do.
- Despite having died, his family was glad that his work was recognized and the discovery _________________________ to him.
- In the _________________________ situation where you were president, what would you do?
- Since he always posted pictures of himself with his children, people thought he was very family _________________________.
Reading Discussion
Read the definition of a key term in the reading and discuss the following questions.
fundamental attribution error (n) an assumption that overemphasizes internal factors as attributions for behavior and underestimates the power of the situation
-
- What could be some of the negative consequences of the fundamental attribution error?
- Which cultures do you think tend to commit the fundamental attribution error more? Explain your reasoning.
- If someone is committing the fundamental attribution error, what advice would you give them?
- Download the original, un-adapted version for free at https://openstax.org/details/books/psychology ↵